NeurIPS 2019
Sun Dec 8th through Sat the 14th, 2019 at Vancouver Convention Center
Paper ID:7684
Title:Using Embeddings to Correct for Unobserved Confounding in Networks

Reviewer 1


		
This work considers causal inference in the presence of unobserved confounding and studies the case where a proxy is available for the unobserved confounding in the form of a network connecting the units. This is an important problem and applying embedding to causal inference seems a natural way nowadays. The novelty lies in using a network proxy to estimate the unobserved confounder. The theoretical results build upon Chernozhukov et al and modified to allow for the use of the full data in the embedding training. The paper writing seems rush to me. There are a few issues/confusion here: 1) Page 3, Section 2. Embedding methods. Anonymous [Ano] 2) Page 3, last paragraph in Section 3, what is \hat{\eta}_n? 3) Page 3, Equation in Example 4.2, what is \sigma?

Reviewer 2


		
Summary: The paper introduces a new method for causal effect estimation by exploiting social network structure to capture possible confounding aspects between treatment and outcome, which can subsequently be adjusted for. It translates a social network into an embedding that can be used as a proxy variables for the actual confounders. It is based on the assumption that closely linked friends in a network are likely to be similar. Performance is evaluated on partially simulated real-world data on various data sets. The subject is interesting and difficult: many tech giants are eager to exploit and extract all kinds of new information from social networks, and this application could offer a very interesting opportunity. However, despite this intriguing backdrop, the paper itself does not manage to convince at all points. The main rationale for using the (social) network is that ‘similar people are likely to be friends’ (l..21). This makes intuitive sense, however it does not imply that people that are far apart in a social network are likely to be very dissimilar. As the resulting similarity measures from the network embeddings are subsequently used as proxy for the actual confounding variables, it would be helpful to provide a slightly more in-depth example + explanation of how effective this proxy actually captures the key confounding aspects of the unobserved variables. At the moment we are required to trust in the ‘black box’ (l.136) … which is ok, but I still would like a bit more reassurance to start from. Unfortunately the current evaluations provided in section 6 are not enough due to the lack of a reliable ground-truth. I applaud the authors for making the assumptions behind their approach very specific (as every principled causal method should do). However, this also lays bare some of the potential weak points behind it. The translation of the network into a predictive embedding that serves as a good proxy for use in a causal inference procedure is interesting and promising. However, then asking a practitioner to assess the plausibility of this predictive embedded model is not realistic in practice. For a generating model s/he will at least have some notion of possible mechanisms to give a good assessment … but for such an abstract embedded model this is almost impossible unless s/he has a huge amount of experience in judging such models, as already acknowledged by the authors (l.209). However, a more important issue is that the current assumptions are either not complete or do not seem to guarantee validity. In particular: if the observed joint density of a straightforward model X <- Z -> (T -> Y), (i.e. X good proxy of confounder Z of causal link from T to Y) follows a multivariate Gaussian distribution, then the system satisfies the assumptions, and adjusting on Z (or X) will indeed lead to a good approximation of the causal effect of T on Y. However, there ALSO exists a linear Gaussian model T -> (Z -> Y) + Z -> X, (i.e. X good proxy of partial mediator Z of causal link between T and Y) that matches the exact same distribution, and for that model/system adjusting on Z or X would clearly lead to a wrong causal prediction. Essentially you require that X and Z are not causally affected by treatment T. But this is an assumption on the underlying model: if satisfied then assumption 1 automatically follows, but not necessarily the other way around. Furthermore, you start from X as a noisy proxy of the actual confounder Z, but then as stated in l.201, Assumption 2 essentially states that X captures ‘whatever information is relevant to the prediction of the treatment and outcome’. That does not correspond to a noisy observation, but essentially still relies on obtaining full information on the effect of the confounding variable. I was hoping for a bound on the accuracy of the estimated causal effect in relation to the ‘closeness’ of the proxy variable X to the actual confounder Z. The hope expressed in l.210-213 only holds if both observed/proxy and unobserved/exogenous confounding have the same sign and the second is not stronger than the first, otherwise the unadjusted estimate may actually be closer to the ground truth. (This also holds for the experiment behind Fig.1). On a final note: I have also had the pleasure of reviewing the closely related ‘Using Text Embeddings for Causal Inference’ which conceptually seemed more interesting than this one. In conclusion: the problem is interesting and highly relevant, and the approach is promising. However, there is some concern about the actual validity in section 5, and the experiments don’t seem sufficiently rigorous to fully capture a proper evaluation of efficacy of the method. Together with the strong overlap with the other paper mentioned above I find this the weaker one, and hence recommend reject.

Reviewer 3


		
# Summary The authors present a method that exploits graph embedding methodology to use graph structure as a proxy in causal estimation problems. They provide sufficient conditions under which adjusting for an embedding learned without reference to a specific generative model will yield asymptotically unbiased and asymptotically normal estimates of causal effects at the parametric rate. They then propose an embedding network architecture that is designed to estimate embeddings that could plausibly satisfy these conditions. They conduct a series of experiments using semisynthetic data to demonstrate how these methods could work in practice. # Feedback This paper was a pleasure to read. The exposition is very clear, assumptions are appropriately foregrounded, and the experiments are designed to test the claims that the authors set out to make. Full disclosure: I reviewed an earlier version of this paper, and am happy to say that it is much improved. ## Elephant in the Room: Assumption 2 My main concern rests with the identifying Assumption 2. It is left unclear how one might reason about this assumption being met in practice. The authors rightly call this out as the crux of the method, and the most difficult assumption to evaluate. However, this discussion still leaves out how one might judge the “plausibility of the predictive embedding model” without falling back to judging a generative model of at least some aspect of the network. In my opinion, assumptions are only as weak as the heuristic by which they can be judged, so I think this question needs to be addressed. One suggestion: the embedding loss defines a set of sufficient statistics of the network that the embedding is designed to reconstruct. I think a condition relating lambda(Z) to these sufficient statistics (for example, a completeness condition stating that unique values of the latent variable map to unique distributions of the sufficient statistics) could be a good starting point. Completeness conditions play a major role in other work on proxy methods. This condition could probably be weakened to focus on the task of predicting treatment or outcomes, but I think there needs to be some formalization of how much confounding information is carried by the observed network. Reasoning about these conditions could also help the user design the Sample() function, which is a major degree of freedom in this approach that could also use some discussion. This being said, the exogenous confounding do a nice job of empirically probing part of this assumption to some extent. However, it doesn’t help the reader to try to reason about how confounding information might be represented in the aspects of the network that are being modeled by the embedding. ## Empirical Experiments A major strength of the paper is the empirical experiments, although, as with all experimental sections, there are more aspects to explore. I have one major concern with the statement that adjusting for the network always helps, even when confounding is not fully explained by the network. This is not generally true for confounder adjustment: it is well-known that adjusting partway for confounders that are highly predictive of treatment but not of outcome can increase bias. This phenomenon is known as bias amplification or Z-bias (see, e.g., Middleton et al https://www.cambridge.org/core/journals/political-analysis/article/bias-amplification-and-bias-unmasking/B95DDA52BE93B761C067EEE60739DDBD, Ding et al https://academic.oup.com/biomet/article/104/2/291/3737784). The simple generating process in this experiment does not admit this behavior because the propensity score enters directly into the prognostic score, so it is not possible to explain variation in the treatment without also explaining a proportional amount of variation in the outcome. I would expect to see this problem arise in an experiment where the network is highly predictive of treatment assignment and much less so of outcome, but this possibility is excluded by design in the experiment. This is not a damning concern, but I would encourage the authors to consider tempering their conclusions on this issue, and to raise the possibility in a discussion of results. Ultimately, one still needs to reason about what information is conveyed by the graph embedding. There is also some amount of control that the user has about what information the embedding will carry based on the weights they put on the two predictive portions of the network. Perhaps this could be suggested as future work. Smaller questions: Why is the parametric model given 128 blocks? I am curious about what would happen if you gave the parametric model three blocks, as I imagine at least the district latent variable being expressed as a block structure in the network. Is it possible to visualize how the user features manifest in the network? For example, does the adjacency matrix appear to have 3 communities determined by district? ## Nitpicks Miao et al does not need the marginal distribution to be identifiable. That is the primary contribution of that paper. For the sufficiency of Q(1, Z), people often cite Hansen 2008 https://academic.oup.com/biomet/article-abstract/95/2/481/230183 as well. In the network ERM objective, does the subgraph always include all of the original nodes, and are you only subsampling edges? If not, what do you do for units that are not included as vertices in the subgraph. ------------------------- # Post-Rebuttal Feedback I am still bullish on this paper because I think it presents an innovative approach to the problem and explains all of its moving parts clearly. I think somebody can read this paper and know exactly why they would dispute conclusions based on the methodology. I am a bit disappointed in the response regarding Assumption 2. I was hoping for a little bit of deeper thought on this issue (especially given that this is the second round of feedback that's focused on this question). I don't find the analogy to image models to be particularly compelling, because we can directly assess the performance of those models against ground truth, but only have our assumptions to rely on in causal inference problems. In fact, there are active debates about the reliability of decision-making that relies on the internal representations of these models. So I think the authors need to reflect a bit more and modify their advice here. I think they need to at least highlight a number of the important factors discussed in the proxies/measurement error literature that determine how well causal effects can be recovered. All of that said, I think this paper still meets the bar for publication. This would also make it easier to have discussions out in the open about the potential pitfalls of using embeddings as proxies for confounding variables, which I think would be useful for the community as a whole.